Details

Title

The relative input of payoffs and probabilities into risk judgment

Journal title

Polish Psychological Bulletin

Yearbook

2010

Numer

No 2

Publication authors

Divisions of PAS

Nauki Humanistyczne i Społeczne

Publisher

Committee for Psychological Science PAS

Date

2010

Identifier

ISSN 0079-2993

References

Brachinger H. (1997), Risk as a primitive: A survey of measures of perceived risk, OR Spektrum, 19, 235, doi.org/10.1007/BF01539781 ; Brandstatter E. (2006), The Priority Heuristic: making choices without trade-offs, Psychological Review, 113, 2, 409, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.409 ; Brigham T. (1979), Choice and Perceived Control. ; Cohen J. (1959), Preferences for Different Combinations of Chance and Skill in Gambling, Namrc, 183, 841. ; Coombs C. (1978), An experimental study of risk preferences in lotteries, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 3, 497, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.4.3.497 ; Coombs C. (1981), Evaluation of two alternative models of a theory of risk: I. Are moment of distributions useful in assessing risk?, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 5, 1110, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.5.1110 ; Coombs C. (1984), Conjoint design analysis of the bilinear model: an application to judgments of risk, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 28, 1, 1, doi.org/10.1016/0022-2496(84)90018-X ; Edwards W. (1954), The theory of decision making, Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380, doi.org/10.1037/h0053870 ; Gonzales R. (1999), On the shape of probability weighting function, Cognitive Psychology, 38, 129, doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0710 ; Heath C. (1991), Preference and Belief: Ambiguity and Competence in Choice under Uncertainty, Journal of Risk and Uncertain, 4, 5, doi.org/10.1007/BF00057884 ; Howell W. (1971), Uncertainty from Internal and External Sources: A Clear Case of Overconfidence, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 2, 240, doi.org/10.1037/h0031206 ; Huber O. (1997), Active information search and complete information presentation in naturalistic risky decision tasks, Acta Psychologica, 95, 15, doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(96)00028-5 ; Johnson E. (2008), Process models deserve process data: Comment on Brandstätter, Gigerenzer, and Hertwig (2006), Psychological Review, 115, 263, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.263 ; Kahneman D. (1972), Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness, Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430, doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3 ; Keller L. (1986), Empirical investigation of some properties of the perceived riskiness of gambles, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 38, 114, doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(86)90029-4 ; Kuhn K. (1996), The relative importance of probabilities, outcomes, and vagueness in hazard risk decisions, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 301, doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0107 ; Kuhn K. (1999), Attribute tradeoffs in low probability / high consequence risks: The joint effects of dimension preference and vagueness, Risk, Decision, and Policy, 4, 31, doi.org/10.1080/135753099348085 ; Luce R. (1986), An axiomatic theory of conjoint, expected risk, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 30, 2, 188, doi.org/10.1016/0022-2496(86)90013-1 ; March J. (1987), Managerial Perspectives on Risk and Risk Taking, Management Science, 33, 11, 1404, doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.33.11.1404 ; Payne J. (1975), Relation of perceived risk to preferences among gambles, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 1, 86, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.1.1.86 ; Payne J. (1976), Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 366, doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(76)90022-2 ; Rotter J. (1966), Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement, Psychological Monographs, 80, 1, doi.org/10.1037/h0092976 ; Shapira Z. (1994), Risk Taking: A managerial perspective. ; Slovic P. (1967), The relative influence of probabilities and payoffs upon perceived risk of a gamble, Psychometric Science, 9, 4, 223, doi.org/10.3758/BF03330840 ; Slovic P. (1968b), Relative importance of probabilities and payoffs in risk taking, Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 78, 3, doi.org/10.1037/h0026468 ; Sokolowska J. (2000), Fairness & Cooperation. ; Tversky A. (1992), Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297, doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574 ; Tversky A. (1983), Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment, Psychological Review, 91, 293, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293 ; Tversky A. (1994), Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability, Psychological Review, 101, 547, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.547 ; Wagenaar W. (1972), Generation of random sequences by human subjects: a critical survey of literature, Psychological Bulletin, 77, 65, doi.org/10.1037/h0032060 ; Weiner B. (1971/72), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. ; Willemsen, M. C., Johnson, E. J. (2006). MouselabWEB: Monitoring information acquisition processes on the Web. Retrieved August 14, 2008, from <a target="_blank" href='http://www.mouselabweb.org/'>http://www.mouselabweb.org/</a>

DOI

10.2478/v10059-010-0006-2

×