Szczegóły

Tytuł artykułu

The Influence of Emotional and Non-emotional Concepts Activation on Information Processing and Unintentional Memorizing

Tytuł czasopisma

Polish Psychological Bulletin

Rocznik

2011

Numer

No 3

Autorzy publikacji

Wydział PAN

Nauki Humanistyczne i Społeczne

Wydawca

Committee for Psychological Science PAS

Data

2011

Identyfikator

ISSN 0079-2993

Referencje

Antos S. (1979), Processing facilitation in a lexical decision task, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 527, doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.527 ; Baumeister R. (2001), Bad is stronger than good, Review of General Psychology, 5, 323, doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 ; Bless H. (1996), Mood and the use of scripts, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.665 ; Bower G. (1981), Mood and memory, American Psychologist, 36, 129, doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.36.2.129 ; Browne B. (2006), Effects of exposure duration on lexical decision, Southeastern Journal of Psychology, 1, 24. ; Cacioppo J. (1999), Emotion, Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191, doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191 ; Cacioppo J. (2001), Between affect and mind. ; Carroll N. (2005), Priming of emotion recognition, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1173. ; Challis B. (1988), Mood induction and the priming of semantic memory in a lexical decision task: asymmetric effect of elation and depression, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 309. ; Claeys W. (1993), Some instantiations of the informational negativity effect: positive - negative asymmetry in category breadth and in estimated meaning similarity of trait adjectives, European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 111, doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420230202 ; Clark D. (1983), Effect of mood on lexical decision, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 175. ; Collins A. (1975), A spreading activation theory of semantic memory, Psychological Review, 82, 407, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 ; Derryberry D. (1994), The heart's eye. Emotional influences in perception and attention. ; Diener E. (1995), The personality structure of affect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.130 ; Z Estes (2008), Automatic vigilance for negative words in lexical decision nad naming: comment on Larsen, Mercer, & Balota (2006), Emotion, 8, 441, doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.441 ; Eysenck M. (2005), Cognitive Psychology: A student's handbook. ; Goetz M. (2007), What's the use of being happy? Mood states, useful objects, and repetition priming effects, Emotion, 7, 675, doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.675 ; Greenwald A. (1996), Three cognitive markers of uncioncious semantic activation, Science, 273, 1699, doi.org/10.1126/science.273.5282.1699 ; Gross, C. I. (2006). <i>Mood congruent memory: effects on a lexical decision task and free recall task.</i> A thesis submitted for the degree of bachelor of science in psychology. The University of British Columbia. ; Kemp-Wheeler S. (1992), Semantic and emotional priming below objective detection threshold, Cognition and Emotion, 6, 113, doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411062 ; Klinger M. (2000), Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition, not spreading activation, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2, 441, doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.441 ; Kolanczyk A. (2003), Psychology of Mind. ; Kuchinke, L (2007). <i>Implicit and explicit recognition of emotionally valenced words.</i> PhD unpublished thesis. <a target="_blank" href='http://www.diss.fr-berlin.de/2007/285'>www.diss.fr-berlin.de/2007/285</a> ; Kuchinke L. (2005), Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: An fMRI study, NeuroImage, 28, 1022, doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.050 ; Nabeta T. (2006), Congruency effect of presentation modality on false recognition of haptic and visual objects, Memory, 14, 307, doi.org/10.1080/09658210500277398 ; McNamara T. (2003), Experimental psychology, 4, 447. ; Neely J. (1989), Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospective prime -generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1003, doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1003 ; Olafson K. (2001), Effects of emotional state on lexical decision performance, Brain and Cognition, 45, 15, doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2000.1248 ; Pawlowska-Fusiara, M. (2005). Asymmetrical influence of positive and negative affect on involuntary memory. Unpublished PhD Dissertation, Warsaw School of Social Sciences and Humanities.[in Polish] ; Perea M. (2002), The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task, Psychological Research, 66, 180, doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0086-5 ; Piercey C. (2008), Inconsistent mood congruent effects in lexical decision experiments, Journal of Articles in Support The Null Hypothesis, 5, 19. ; Plaut D. (2000), Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing, Psychological Review, 4, 786, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.4.786 ; Pratto F. (1991), Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380 ; Ratcliff R. (2004), A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task, Psychological Review, 111, 159, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.159 ; Ratcliff R. (2001), A multinomial model for short-term priming in word identification, Psychological Review, 108, 835, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.835 ; Ray S. (2007), Investigating long-term semantic priming of middle- and low-familiarity category exemplars, The Journal of General Psychology, 134, 453, doi.org/10.3200/GENP.134.4.453-466 ; Ray S. (2008), Two routes for activation in the priming of categorical coordinates, The Journal of General Psychology, 135, 65, doi.org/10.3200/GENP.135.1.65-83 ; Robinson-Riegler G. (1996), The role of conscious recollection in recognition of affective material. Evidence for positive-negative asymmetry, The Journal of General Psychology, 123, 93, doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1996.9921263 ; Rossel S. (2004), Semantic priming of different affective categories, Emotion, 4, 354, doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.354 ; Scott K. (2001), Masked semantic priming of emotional information in subclinical depression, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 505, doi.org/10.1023/A:1005541701832 ; Siegle, G. J. (1996). <i>Rumination on affect: Cause for negative attention biases in depression.</i> Unpublished master thesis. San Diego University. San Diego CA. ; Stip E. (1994), Influence of affective words on lexical decision task in major depression, Journal of Psychiatric Neuroscience, 19, 202. ; Thomas L. (2005), Emotional arousal enhances word repetition priming, Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1027, doi.org/10.1080/02699930500172440 ; Unkelbach Ch. (2008), Why positive information is processed faster: The density hypothesis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 36, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.36 ; Wentura D. (2005), Repeated masked category primes interfere with related exemplars: New evidence for negative semantic priming, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 108, doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.108 ; Wentura D. (2000), Dissociative affective and associative priming effects in the lexical decision task: Yes versus no responses to word targets reveal evaluative judgment tendencies, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and Cognition, 26, 456, doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.456 ; Winkielman P. (2007), Affective influence on judgments and decision: Moving towards core mechanisms, Review of General Psychology, 11, 179, doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.179

DOI

10.2478/v10059-011-0020-z

×