Details

Title

No Spearman’s Law of Diminishing Returns for the working memory and intelligence relationship

Journal title

Polish Psychological Bulletin

Yearbook

2016

Numer

No 1

Publication authors

Divisions of PAS

Nauki Humanistyczne i Społeczne

Publisher

Committee for Psychological Science PAS

Date

2016

Identifier

ISSN 0079-2993

References

Halford (2005), How many variables can humans process, Psychological Science, 16, 70, doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00782.x ; Oberauer (2005), Working memory and intelligence - their correlation and their relation : Comment on Ackerman, Psychological Bulletin, 131. ; Carpenter (1990), What one intelligence test measures : A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices test, Psychological Review, 97. ; Der (2003), IQ , reaction time and the differentiation hypothesis, Intelligence, 31, 491, doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00189-7 ; McGrew (2009), CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project : Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research, Intelligence, 37, 1, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004 ; Detterman (1989), Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups, Intelligence, 13, 349, doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(89)80007-8 ; Hartmann (2006), Spearman s Law of Diminishing Returns tested with two methods, Intelligence, 34, 47, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.06.002 ; Kane (2005), Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs : Comment on Ackerman, Psychological Bulletin, 131. ; Burgess (2011), Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span of :, Journal Experimental Psychology General, 140. ; Jauk (2013), The relationship between intelligence and creativity : New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection, Intelligence, 41, 212, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003 ; Halford (1998), Processing capacity defined by relational complexity : Implications for comparative , developmental , and cognitive psychology and, Behavioral Brain Sciences, 21, 803, doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98001769 ; Oberauer (2008), Which working memory functions predict intelligence, Intelligence, 36, 641, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.007 ; Deary (2012), Intelligence In pp, Annual review of psychology, 453, doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353 ; Cowan (2006), Scope of attention , control of attention , and intelligence in children and adults Memory, Cognition, 34, 1754, doi.org/10.3758/BF03195936 ; Ragni (2013), A theory and a computational model of spatial reasoning with preferred mental models, Psychological Review, 120. ; Goodwin (2005), Reasoning about relations, Psychological Review, 112. ; Vigneau (2006), Eye - movement analysis demonstrates strategic influence on intelligence, Intelligence, 34, 261, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.003 ; MacCallum (2002), On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables, Psychological Methods, 7. ; Evans (1999), On the asymmetry of g, Psychological Reports, 85. ; Reynolds (2010), Use of factor mixture modeling to capture Spearman s law of diminishing returns, Intelligence, 38, 231, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.01.002 ; Martínez (2011), Can fl uid intelligence be reduced to simple short - term storage, Intelligence, 39. ; Hummel (2003), A symbolic - connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization, Psychological Review, 110, 220, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.220 ; Unsworth (2011), Attention control and the antisaccade task A response time distribution analysis, Acta Psychologica, 137. ; Colom (2008), Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs but why, Intelligence, 36, 584, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002 ; Cleveland (1981), LOWESS : A program for smoothing scatterplots by robust locally weighted regression The, American Statistician, 35. ; Jensen (2003), Regularities in Spearman s law of diminishing returns, Intelligence, 31, 95, doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00094-0 ; Chuderski (2015), From neural oscillations to complex cognition : Simulating the effect of the theta - to - gamma cycle length ratio on analogical reasoning, Cognitive Psychology, 76. ; Reynolds (2007), Spearman s law of diminishing returns in hierarchical models of intelligence for children and adolescents, Intelligence, 35, 267, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.002 ; Chuderski (2014), Relational integration task explains fluid reasoning above and beyond other working memory tasks Memory, Cognition, 42, 448, doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0366-x ; Chuderski (2013), When are fluid intelligence and working memory isomorphic and when are they not, Intelligence, 41, 244, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.003 ; Cowan (2001), The magical number in short - term memory : A reconsideration of mental storage capacity and, Behavioral Brain Sciences, 24, 87, doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01003922 ; Kane (2002), The role of prefrontal cortex in working - memory capacity , executive attention , and general fluid intelligence : An individual - differences perspective, Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 9, 637, doi.org/10.3758/BF03196323 ; Unsworth (2010), Lapses in sustained attention and their relation to executive control and fluid abilities : An individual differences investigation, Intelligence, 38, 111, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.08.002 ; Chuderski (2012), Storage capacity explains fl uid intelligence while executive control does not, Intelligence, 40, 278, doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.02.010 ; Akaike (1974), A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification on, IEEE Transactions Automatic Control, 19, 716, doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 ; Hamel (2006), The - minute version as a predictor of The Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices Test and, Educational Psychological Measurement, 66.

DOI

10.1515/ppb-2016-0008

×