Szczegóły

Tytuł artykułu

The Under— and Overestimation Effects in Comparative Judgments — Assimilation and Contrast Mechanisms

Tytuł czasopisma

Polish Psychological Bulletin

Rocznik

2008

Numer

No 4

Autorzy

Wydział PAN

Nauki Humanistyczne i Społeczne

Wydawca

Committee for Psychological Science PAS

Data

2008

Identyfikator

ISSN 0079-2993

Referencje

Alicke M. (1995), Personal contact, individuation, and the Better-Than Average-Effect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 804, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804 ; Chambers J. (2004), Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of non-motivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects, Psychological Bulletin, 130, 813, doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813 ; Chambers J. (2003), Egocentrism, event frequency, and comparative optimism: When what happens frequently is more likely to happen to me, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1343, doi.org/10.1177/0146167203256870 ; Codol J. (1989), Asymmetry in the estimation of interpersonal distance and identity affirmation, European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 11, doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420190103 ; Cypryańska M. (2005), Nierealistyczny optymizm - optymizm przewidywań czy zniekształcenia poznawcze przypadkiem optymistyczne? (Unrealistic optimism - the optimism of predictions or the cognitive biases that are optimistic by a chance?), Studia Psychologiczne, 43, 37. ; Dunning D. (1996), Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 213, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.213 ; Fox C. (2000), Familiarity bias and belief reversal in relative likelihood judgment, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 268, doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2000.2898 ; Hamilton D. (1996), Perceiving persons and groups, Psychological Review, 103, 336, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.336 ; Helveg-Larsen M. (2001), Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 51, 74, doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_5 ; Klar, Y. (2002). Way beyond compare: Nonselective superiority and inferiority biases in judging randomly assig ; ed group members relative to their peers. <i>Journal of Experimental Social Psychology</i>, 38, 331-351. ; Klar Y. (1997), No one in my group can be below the group's average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 885, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.885 ; Klayman J. (1987), Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing, Psychological Review, 94, 211, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.211 ; Kruger J. (1999), Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 221, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.221 ; Kruger J. (2004), Egocentrism and focalism in unrealistic optimism (and pessimism), Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 332, doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.06.002 ; McConnell A. (1997), Target entitativity: Implications for information processing about individual and group targets, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 750, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.750 ; Mussweiler T. (2003), Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences, Psychological Review, 110, 472, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.472 ; Perloff L. (1986), Self-other judgments and perceived invulnerability to victimization, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 502, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.502 ; Srull T. (1983), General principles and individual differences in the self as a habitual reference point: An examination of self-other judgments of similarity, Social Cognition, 2, 108, doi.org/10.1521/soco.1983.2.2.108 ; Tversky A. (1977), Features of similarity, Psychological Review, 84, 327, doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.4.327 ; Tversky A. (1978), Cognition and categorization, 81. ; Weinstein N. (1980), Unrealistic optimism about future life events, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806 ; Weinstein N. (1982), Egocentrism as a source of unrealistic optimism, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 195, doi.org/10.1177/0146167282082002 ; Wilson T. (2000), Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.821 ; Windschitl P. (2003), The influence of egocentrism and focalism on people's optimism in competitions: When what affects us equally affects me more, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 389, doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.389

DOI

10.2478/v10059-008-0027-2

×