Details

Title

A Notional Level of Cognitive Distortions in Depression: Does It Exist? A Voice for Interdisciplinarity in Studying Cognitive Functioning of Individuals with Depressive Disorders

Journal title

Polish Psychological Bulletin

Yearbook

2009

Numer

No 4

Authors

Divisions of PAS

Nauki Humanistyczne i Społeczne

Publisher

Committee for Psychological Science PAS

Date

2009

Identifier

ISSN 0079-2993

References

Addis D. (2006), Prefrontal and hippocampal contributions to the generation and binding of semantic associations during successful encoding, NeuroImage, 33, 1194. ; Andreasen N. (2003), Fascynujący mózg. Walka z chorobami psychicznymi w epoce genomu. ; Atchley R. (2007), The right hemisphere's contribution to emotional word processing in currently depressed, remitted depressed, and never-depressed individuals, Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 145. ; Barnett K. (2008), Familial patterns and the origins of individual differences in synaesthesia, Cognition, 106, 871. ; Bartczak M. (2008), Metaphorical conceptualization of some notions in depressive disorders: Is PLEASURE an insipid milky jelly?, Psychology of Language and Communication, 12, 1, 85. ; Bartczak M. (2009), A new tool investigating metaphorical conceptualization of some notions: May it differentiate people in the area of mood?, null. ; Beck A. (1963), Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions, Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324. ; Beck A. (1967), Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. ; Beck A. (1973), The diagnosis and management of depression. ; Beck A. (1976), Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. ; Beck A. (1995), Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. ; Beeman M. (1998), Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, 255. ; Blasko D. (1999), Only the tip of the iceberg: Who understands what about metaphor?, Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1675. ; Boroditsky L. (2000), Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors, Cognition, 75, 1. ; Boroditsky L. (2001), Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time, Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1. ; Butler C. (2008), Cognitive adequacy in structural-functional theories of language, Language Sciences, 30, 1. ; Casasanto D. (2008), Time in the mind: Using space to think about time, Cognition, 106, 579. ; Castaneda A. (2008), Cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of young adults with a history of non-psychotic unipolar depressive disorders without psychiatric comorbidity, Journal of Affective Disorders, 110, 1-2, 36. ; Chatterjee A. (2001), Language and space: Some interactions, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 55. ; Chemero A. (2001), Making space for embodiment [review of the book <i>Representation and behavior</i>], Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 317. ; Chiao J. (2004), Mental representations of social status, Cognitive Psychology, 22, 184. ; Chiappe D. (2007), The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension, Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 172. ; Clark A. (2006), Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 371. ; Crawford L. (2000), Linguistic and nonlinguistic spatial categorization, Cognition, 75, 209. ; Eichstedt J. (1998), Metaphorical gender knowledge in the second year of life, Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 394. ; Eichstedt J. (2002), Of bears and men: Infants' knowledge of conventional and metaphorical gender stereotypes, Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 296. ; L. Feldman Barrett (2007), Cognitive-emotional interactions: Language as context for the perception of emotion, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 327. ; Feldman G. (2006), From molecules to metaphor: A neural theory of language. ; Feldman J. (2004), Embodied meaning in a neural theory of language, Brain and Language, 89, 385. ; Fodor J. (1975), The language of thought. ; Fossati P. (1999), Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia, Psychiatry Researches, 89, 171. ; Foster P. (2008), Spreading activation of lexical-semantic networks in Parkinson's disease, Neuropsychologia, 46, 1908. ; Gallistel C. (2002), Language and spatial frames of reference in mind and brain, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 321. ; Gennari S. (2002), Motion events in language and cognition, Cognition, 83, 49. ; Gentner D. (2001), Convention, form, and figurative language processing, Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 223. ; Gentner D. (2001), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science, 199. ; Georgieff N. (1998), Semantic priming in major depressive state, Psychiatry Research, 78, 29. ; Gernsbacher M. (2001), The role of suppression and enhancement in understanding metaphors, Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 433. ; Gibbs R. (2006), Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation, Mind & Language, 21, 434. ; Giora R. (1997), Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis, Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 183. ; Giora R. (2002), Literal vs. figurative language: Different or equal?, Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 487. ; Giora R. (2000), Differential effects of right- and left-hemisphere damage on understanding sarcasm and metaphor, Metaphor and Symbol, 1, 63. ; Glucksberg S. (2001), Understanding figurative language: From metaphors to idioms. ; Glucksberg S. (2003), The psycholinguistics of metaphor, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 92. ; Gollan T. (2006), Word association in early Alzheimer's disease, Brain and Language, 99, 289. ; Gotlib I. (2000), Stress, coping, and depression, 117. ; Grice H. (1975), Speech acts: Syntax and Semantics, 41. ; Grossenbacher P. (2001), Mechanisms of synesthesia: Cognitive and physiological constraints, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 36. ; Hayek F. (1952), The sensory order. ; Hebb D. (1949), The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. ; Holmes E. (2005), Mental imagery and emotion: A special relationship?, Emotion, 5, 489. ; Holmes E. (2006), Positive interpretation training: Effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood, Behavior Therapy, 37, 237. ; Johnson C. (1999), Cultural, psychological and typological issues in cognitive linguistics, 155, doi.org/10.1075/cilt.152.12joh ; Johnson M. (1992), Philosophical implications of cognitive semantics, Cognitive Linguistics, 3-4, 28. ; Jones L. (2005), Metaphor comprehension as attributive categorization, Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 110. ; Joorman J. (2006), Podpatrywanie myśli i uczuć. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja procesów emocjonalnych i poznawczych. Nowe kierunki badań, 148. ; Jung-Beeman M. (2005), Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 512. ; Just M. (1992), A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory, Psychological Review, 99, 122. ; Kay P. (1978), The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms, Language, 54, 3, 610. ; Keysar B. (2000), Conventional language: How metaphorical is it?, Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 576. ; Kintsch W. (2000), Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 257, doi.org/10.3758/BF03212981 ; Kintsch W. (2001), Predication, Cognitive Science, 25, 173. ; Kravchenko A. (2006), Cognitive linguistics, biology of cognition, and biosemiotics: Bridging the gaps, Language Sciences, 28, 51. ; Lakoff G. (1987), Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories tell us about the nature of thought, doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 ; Lakoff G. (1993), Metaphor and thought, 202, doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173865.013 ; Lakoff G. (1980a), Metaphors we live by. ; Lakoff G. (1980b), The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system, Cognitive Science, 4, 195. ; Lakoff G. (1999), Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. ; Lamberton A. (2008), A test of the cognitive content specificity hypothesis in depression and anxiety, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 23. ; Levinson S. (2002), Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning, Cognition, 84, 155. ; Li P. (2002), Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning, Cognition, 83, 265. ; Libura A. (2000), Wyobraźnia w języku: Leksykalne korelaty schematów wyobrażeniowych: Centrum-Peryferie i Siły. ; Mahurin R. (2006), Trail making test errors and executive function in schizophrenia and depression, Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 271. ; Markošová M. (2008), Network model of human language, Physica A, 387, 661. ; Mashal N. (2008), Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: Application of the signal detection theory, Brain and Language, 104, 103. ; Mashal N. (2007), An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions, Brain and Language, 100, 115. ; McNally R. (2006), Cognitive abnormalities in post-traumatic stress disorder, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 271. ; Monetta L. (2007), Effects of verbal working memory deficits on metaphor comprehension in patients with Parkinson's disease, Brain and Language, 101, 80. ; Mulvenna C. (2006), Synaesthesia: Supernormal integration?, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 350. ; Munnich E. (2001), Spatial language and spatial representation: A cross-linguistic comparison, Cognition, 81, 171. ; Murphy G. (1996), On metaphoric representation, Cognition, 60, 173. ; Pinker S. (1994), The language instinct, doi.org/10.1037/e412952005-009 ; Pużyński S. (2002), Depresje i zaburzenia afektywne. ; (1997), Klasyfikacja zaburzeń psychicznych i zaburzeń zachowania w ICD-10. Opisy kliniczne i wskazówki diagnostyczne. ; Raes F. (2009), Reducing cognitive vulnerability to depression: A preliminary investigation of MEmory Specificity Training (MEST) in inpatients with depressive symptomatology, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 1, 24. ; Rapp A. (2004), Neural correlates of metaphor processing, Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 395. ; Rapp A. (2007), Laterality in metaphor processing: Lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory, Brain and Language, 100, 142. ; Rosenhan D. (2003), Psychopatologia. ; Ross T. (2007), The reliability and validity of qualitative scores for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 475. ; Schlesinger M. (2001), Building a better baby: Embodied models of infant cognition, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 139. ; Schmidt G. (2007), Right hemisphere metaphor processing? Characterizing the lateralization of semantic processes, Brain and Language, 100, 127. ; Schnitzer M. (2005), A neuropsychological theory of metaphor, Language Sciences, 27, 31. ; Searle J. (1979), Metaphor and thought, 91. ; Seitz J. (2000), The bodily basis of thought, New Ideas in Psychology, 18, 23. ; Seitz J. (2005), The neural, evolutionary, developmental, and bodily basis of metaphor, New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 74. ; Sheline Y. (2006), Cognitive function in late life depression: Relationships to depression severity, cerebrovascular risk factors and processing speed, Biological Psychiatry, 60, 58. ; Shibata M. (2007), Neural mechanisms involved in the comprehension of metaphoric and literal sentences: An fMRI study, Brain Research, 1166, 92. ; Smith D. (2006), Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic young adults with bipolar spectrum disorder and recurrent major depressive disorder, Bipolar Disorder, 8, 40. ; Solomon A. (2005), Psychoterapia poznawcza, 25. ; Stępnik K. (1988), Filozofia metafory. ; Sternberg D. (1976), Memory functions in depression, Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 2, 219, doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770020055009 ; Stringaris A. (2006), How metaphors influence semantic relatedness judgments: The role of the right frontal cortex, Neuro-Image, 33, 784. ; Tendahl M. (2008), Complementary perspectives on metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory, Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 11, 1823. ; Thibodeau P. (2008), Productive figurative communication: Conventional metaphors facilitate the comprehension of related novel metaphors, Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 521. ; Thompson E. (2001), Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 418. ; Turner M. (1993), Conceptualization and mental processing in language. ; F. Van der Gucht (2007), The iconicity of embodied meaning. Polysemy of spatial prepositions in the cognitive framework, Language Sciences, 29, 733. ; Vigliocco G. (2004), From mind in the mouth to language in the mind [Review of the book <i>Language in Mind</i>], Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 5. ; U. von Hecker (1999), Uncontrollability, depression, and the construction of mental models: A meta-analysis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 833. ; Walter H. (2003), Content, context and cognitive style in mood-memory interactions. Response to Lewis and Critchley, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 433. ; Ward J. (2003), Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: Linguistic and conceptual factors, Cognition, 89, 237. ; Whorf B. (1956), Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. ; Wilson D. (2004), Truthfulness and relevance, Mind, 111, 583. ; Xu F. (2007), Sortal concepts, object individuation, and language, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 400. ; Yu N. (2004), The eyes for sight and mind, Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 663.

DOI

10.2478/s10059-009-0016-0

×