Search results

Filters

  • Journals
  • Authors
  • Keywords
  • Date
  • Type

Search results

Number of results: 18
items per page: 25 50 75
Sort by:
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This paper has two distinct parts. Section 1 includes general discussion of the phenomenon of "absolute pitch" (AP), and presentation of various concepts concerning definitions of "full", "partial" and "pseudo" AP. Sections 2-4 include presentation of the experiment concerning frequency range in which absolute pitch appears, and discussion of the experimental results. The experiment was performed with participation of 9 AP experts selected from the population of 250 music students as best scoring in the pitch-naming piano-tone screening tests. Each subject had to recognize chromas of 108 pure tones representing the chromatic musical scale of nine octaves from E0 to D#9. The series of 108 tones was presented to each subject 60 times in random order, diotically, with loudness level about 65 phon. Percentage of correct recognitions (PC) for each tone was computed. The frequency range for the existence of absolute pitch in pure tones, perceived by sensitive AP possessors stretches usually over 5 octaves from about 130.6 Hz (C3) to about 3.951 Hz (B7). However, it was noted that in a single case, the upper boundary of AP was 9.397 Hz (D9). The split-halves method was applied to estimate the reliability of the obtained results.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Andrzej Rakowski
Piotr Rogowski
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Absolute pitch is a unique feature of the auditory memory which makes it possible for its possessors to recognize the musical name (chroma) of a tone. Six musicians with absolute pitch, selected from a group of 250 music students as best scoring in musical pitch-naming tests, identified the chroma of residue pitch produced by harmonic complex tones with several lower partials removed (residual sounds). The data show that the percentage of correct chroma recognitions decreases as the lowest physically existent harmonic in the spectrum is moved higher. According to our underlying hypothesis the percentage of correct chroma recognitions corresponds to the pitch strength of the investigated tones. The present results are compared with pitch strength values derived in an experiment reported by Houtsma and Smurzynski (1990) for tones same as those used in this study but investigated with the use of a different method which consisted in identification of musical intervals between two successive tones. For sounds comprising only harmonics of very high order the new method yields a very low pitch recognition level of about 20% while identification of musical intervals remains stable at a level of about 60%.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Piotr Rogowski
Andrzej Rakowski
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This work addresses the problem of difficulties in classical interpretation of combination tones as non- linear distortions. One of the basic problems of such an interpretation is to point out the sources of these distortions. Besides, these kinds of distortions have numerous “anomalies” which are difficult to explain on the grounds of physics or physiology. The aim of the model presented in this paper is to show that combination tones phenomenon can be explained as an effect of central mechanisms. Most of existing theories of pitch perception focus mainly on virtual pitch perception and do not take into account combination tones as an element of the same mechanism. The proposed model of central auditory processing for pitch perception allows one to interpret in a coherent way both virtual pitches and combination tones phenomena. This model is of a demonstrative nature and gives an introduction to more advanced model. It belongs to the class of spectral models and it will be shown that such a model can be in a simple way extended to spectral - time model which is partially consistent with autocorrelation models.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Tadeusz Ziębakowski
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The lower limit of pitch (LLP) perception was explored for pure tones, sinusoidally amplitude-modulated (SAM) tones with a carrier frequency of 125 Hz, and trains of 125-Hz tone pips, using an adaptive procedure to estimate the lowest repetition rate for which a tonal/humming quality was heard. The LLP was similar for the three stimulus types, averaging 19 Hz. There were marked individual differences, which were correlated to some extent across stimulus types. The pure-tone stimuli contained a single resolved harmonic, whereas the SAM tones and tone-pip trains contained only unresolved components, whose frequencies did not necessarily form a harmonic series. The similarity of the LLP across stimulus types suggests that the LLP is determined by the repetition period of the sound for pure tones, and the envelope repetition period for complex stimuli. The results are consistent with the idea that the LLP is determined by a periodicity analysis in the auditory system, and that the longest time interval between waveform or envelope peaks for which this analysis can be performed is approximately 53 ms.
Go to article

Bibliography

1. Atal B.S. (1972), Automatic speaker recognition based on pitch contours, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 52(6B): 1687–1697, doi: 10.1121/1.1913303.
2. Bernstein J.G., Oxenham A.J. (2005), An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117(6): 3816–3831, doi: 10.1121/1.1904268.
3. British Society of Audiology (2018), Recommended Procedure: Pure-tone air-conduction and bone conduction threshold audiometry with and without masking, British Society of Audiology, Reading, UK.
4. Burke S. (1998), Missing values, outliers, robust statistics & non-parametric methods, Valid Analytical Measurement Bulletin, 19: 22–27.
5. Carney L.H., Yin T.C.T. (1988), Temporal coding of resonances by low-frequency auditory nerve fibers: Single-fiber responses and a population model, Journal of Neurophysiology, 60(5): 1653–1677, doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.60.5.1653.
6. de Cheveigné A. (1997), Concurrent vowel identification III: A neural model of harmonic interference cancellation, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101(5): 2857–2865, doi: 10.1121/1.419480.
7. de Cheveigné A., Pressnitzer D. (2006), The case of the missing delay lines: Synthetic delays obtained by cross-channel phase interaction, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(6): 3908–3918, doi: 10.1121/1.2195291.
8. Cullen J.K., Long G.R. (1986), Rate discrimination of high-pass-filtered pulse trains, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 79(1): 114–119, doi: 10.1121/1.393762.
9. Dau T. (2003), The importance of cochlear processing for the formation of auditory brainstem and frequency following responses, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(2): 936–950, doi: 10.1121/1.1534833.
10. Drugman T., Huybrechts G., Klimkov, V., Moinet A. (2018), Traditional machine learning for pitch detection, IEEE Signal Processing Letters, 25(11): 1745–1749, doi: 10.1109/LSP.2018.2874155.
11. Drullman R., Festen J.M., Plomp R. (1994), Effect of reducing slow temporal modulations on speech reception, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(5): 2670–2680, doi: 10.1121/1.409836.
12. Elliott T.M., Theunissen F.E. (2009), The modulation transfer function for speech intelligibility, PLOS Computational Biology, 5: e1000302, doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000302.
13. Fastl H. (1983), Fluctuation strength of modulated tones and broadband noise, [in:] Hearing – Physiological Bases and Psychophysics, Klinke R., Hartmann R. [Eds], pp. 282–288, Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-69257-4_41.
14. Fukushima M., Doyle A.M., Mullarkey M.P., Mishkin M., Averbeck B.B. (2015), Distributed acoustic cues for caller identity in macaque vocalization, Royal Society Open Science, 2(12): 150432, doi: 10.1098/rsos.150432.
15. Gerson A., Goldstein J.L. (1978), Evidence for a general template in central optimal processing for pitch of complex tones, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63(2): 498–510, doi: 10.1121/1.381750.
16. Glasberg B.R., Moore B.C.J. (1990), Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data, Hearing Research, 47(1–2): 103–138, doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(90)90170-t.
17. Guttman N., Pruzansky S. (1962), Lower limits of pitch and musical pitch, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 5(3): 207–214, doi: 10.1044/jshr.0503.207.
18. Han K., Wang D. (2014), Neural networks for supervised pitch tracking in noise, [in:] Proceedings of the International Conference on Acoustic, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP), Florence, Italy, pp. 1488–1492, doi: 10.1109/ICASSP.2014.6853845.
19. He C., Trainor L.J. (2009), Finding the pitch of the missing fundamental in infants, Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24): 7718–7722, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0157-09.2009.
20. Hoeschele M. (2017), Animal pitch perception: melodies and harmonies, Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 12: 5–18, doi: 10.3819/CCBR.2017.120002.
21. Hoke M., Ross B., Wickesberg R., Lütkenhöner B. (1984), Weighted averaging – theory and application to electric response audiometry, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 57(5): 484–489, doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(84)90078-6.
22. ISO-226 (2003), Acoustics – normal equal-loudness contours, International Organization for Standardization, Geneva, Switzerland.
23. Jackson H.M., Moore B.C.J. (2013), The dominant region for the pitch of complex tones with low fundamental frequencies, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(2): 1193–1204, doi: 10.1121/1.4812754.
24. Joly O., Baumann S., Poirier C., Patterson R.D., Thiele A., Griffiths T.D. (2014), A perceptual pitch boundary in a non-human primate, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 998, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00998.
25. Jurado C., Gallegos P., Gordillo D., Moore B.C.J. (2017), The detailed shapes of equal-loudness-level contours at low frequencies, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(6): 3821–3832, doi: 10.1121/1.5018428.
26. Jurado C., Larrea M., Patel H., Marquardt T. (2020), Dependency of threshold and loudness on sound duration at low and infrasonic frequencies, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 148(2): 1030–1038, doi: 10.1121/10.0001760 .
27. Jurado C., Marquardt T. (2016), The effect of the helicotrema on low-frequency loudness perception, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 140(5): 3799–3809, doi: 10.1121/1.4967295.
28. Jurado C., Marquardt T. (2020), Brain’s frequency following responses to low-frequency and infrasound, Archives of Acoustics, 45(2): 313–319, doi: 10.24425/aoa.2020.133151.
29. Jurado C., Moore B.C.J. (2010), Frequency selectivity for frequencies below 100 Hz: Comparisons with mid-frequencies, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128(6): 3585–3596, doi: 10.1121/1.3504657.
30. Jurado C., Pedersen C.S., Moore B.C.J. (2011), Psychophysical tuning curves for frequencies below 100 Hz, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(5): 3166–3180, doi: 10.1121/1.3560535.
31. Kanedera N., Arai T., Hermansky H., Pavel M. (1999), On the relative importance of various components of the modulation spectrum for automatic speech recognition, Speech Communication, 28(1): 43–55, doi: 10.1016/S0167-6393(99)00002-3.
32. Kinsler L.E., Frey A.R., Coppens A.B., Sanders J.V. (1999), Fundamentals of Acoustics, 4th ed., New York: Wiley-VCH.
33. Koumura T., Terashima H., Furukawa S. (2019), Cascaded tuning to amplitude modulation for natural sound recognition, Journal of Neuroscience, 39(28): 5517–5533, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2914-18.2019.
34. Krumbholz K., Patterson R.D., Pressnitzer D. (2000), The lower limit of pitch as determined by rate discrimination, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108(3): 1170–1180, doi: 10.1121/1.1287843.
35. Kühler R., Fedtke T., Hensel J. (2015), Infrasonic and low-frequency insert earphone hearing threshold, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(4): EL347–EL353, doi: 10.1121/1.4916795.
36. Logos Foundation (2016), Instrument frequencies and ranges, https://www.logosfoundation.org/kursus/frequen cy_table.html (date last viewed: 05-Oct-20).
37. Marquardt T., Hensel J., Mrowinski D., Scholz G. (2007), Low-frequency characteristics of human and guinea pig cochleae, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(6): 3628–3638, doi: 10.1121/1.2722506.
38. Meddis R., O’Mard L. (1997), A unitary model of pitch perception, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(3): 1811–1820, doi: 10.1121/1.420088.
39. Mehta A.H., Oxenham A.J. (2020), Effect of lowest harmonic rank on fundamental-frequency difference limens varies with fundamental frequency, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(4): 2314– 2322, doi: 10.1121/10.0001092.
40. Mrller H., Pedersen C.S. (2004), Hearing at low and infrasonic frequencies, Noise and Health, 6(23): 37–57.
41. Moore B.C.J. (1982), An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 2nd ed., London: Academic Press.
42. Moore B.C.J. (2008), The role of temporal fine structure processing in pitch perception, masking, and speech perception for normal-hearing and hearingimpaired people, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 9(4): 399–406, doi: 10.1007/s10162-008-0143-x.
43. Moore B.C.J. (2019), The roles of temporal envelope and fine structure information in auditory perception, Acoustical Science and Technology, 40(2): 61–83, doi: 10.1250/ast.40.61.
44. Moore B.C.J., Glasberg B.R., Flanagan H.J., Adams J. (2006), Frequency discrimination of complex tones; assessing the role of component resolvability and temporal fine structure, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(1): 480–490, doi: 10.1121/1.2139070.
45. Moore B.C.J., Glasberg B.R., Low K.E., Cope T., Cope W. (2006), Effects of level and frequency on the audibility of partials in inharmonic complex tones, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120(2): 934–944, doi: 10.1121/1.2216906.
46. Moore B.C.J., Gockel H.E. (2011), Resolvability of components in complex tones and implications for theories of pitch perception, Hearing Research, 276(1–2): 88–97, doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.01.003.
47. Moore B.C.J., Hopkins K., Cuthbertson S. (2009), Discrimination of complex tones with unresolved components using temporal fine structure information, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(5): 3214–3222, doi: 10.1121/1.3106135.
48. Moore B.C.J., Ohgushi K. (1993), Audibility of partials in inharmonic complex tones, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 93(1): 452–461, doi: 10.1121/1.405625.
49. Moore G.A., Moore B.C.J. (2003), Perception of the low pitch of frequency-shifted complexes, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(2): 977– 985, doi: 10.1121/1.1536631.
50. Oxenham A.J. (2008), Pitch perception and auditory stream segregation: implications for hearing loss and cochlear implants, Trends in Amplification, 12(4): 316– 331, doi: 10.1177/1084713808325881.
51. Patterson R.D. (1987), A pulse ribbon model of monaural phase perception, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 82(5): 1560–1586, doi: 10.1121/1.395146.
52. Plomp R. (1964), The ear as a frequency analyzer, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 36(9): 1628–1636, doi: 10.1121/1.1919256.
53. Plomp R. (1967), Pitch of complex tones, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 41(6): 1526–1533, doi: 10.1121/1.1910515.
54. Plomp R. (1983), The role of modulation in hearing, [in:] Hearing – Physiological Bases and Psychophysics, Klinke R., Hartmann R. [Eds], Springer, Berlin, pp. 270–276.
55. Pressnitzer D., Patterson R.D., Krumbholz K. (2001), The lower limit of melodic pitch, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(5): 2074–2084, doi: 10.1121/1.1359797.
56. Ritsma R.J. (1962), Existence region of the tonal residue – I, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34(9A): 1224–1229, doi: 10.1121/1.1918307.
57. Santurette S., Dau T. (2011), The role of temporal fine structure information for the low pitch of highfrequency complex tones, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(1): 282–292, doi: 10.1121/1.3518718.
58. Seebeck A. (1841), Observations on some conditions of tone formation [in German: Beobachtungen über einige Bedingungen der Entstehung von Tönen], Annalen der Physik, 129(7): 417–436, doi: 10.1002/andp.18411290702.
59. Shackleton T.M., Carlyon R.P. (1994), The role of resolved and unresolved harmonics in pitch perception and frequency modulation discrimination, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(6): 3529–3540, doi: 10.1121/1.409970.
60. Shamma S., Dutta K. (2019), Spectro-temporal templates unify the pitch percepts of resolved and unresolved harmonics, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 145(2): 615–629, doi: 10.1121/1.5088504.
61. Singh N.C., Theunissen F.E. (2003), Modulation spectra of natural sounds and ethological theories of auditory processing, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114(6): 3394–3411, doi: 10.1121/1.1624067.
62. Spetner N.B., Olsho L.W. (1990), Auditory frequency resolution in human infancy, Child Development, 61(3): 632–652, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02808.x.
63. Terhardt E. (1974), Pitch, consonance, and harmony, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55(5): 1061–1069, doi: 10.1121/1.1914648.
64. Tichko P., Skoe E. (2017), Frequency-dependent fine structure in the frequency-following response: The byproduct of multiple generators, Hearing Research, 348: 1–15, doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.01.014.
65. Tukey J.W. (1977), Exploratory Data Analysis, Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley Pub.
66. Varnet L., Ortiz-Barajas M.C., Erra R.G., Gervain J., Lorenzi C. (2017), A cross-linguistic study of speech modulation spectra, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(4): 1976–1989, doi: 10.1121/1.5006179.
67. Walker K.M.M., Schnupp J.W.H., Hart-Schnupp S.M.B., King A.J., Bizley J.K. (2009), Pitch discrimination by ferrets for simple and complex sounds, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(3): 1321–1335, doi: 10.1121/1.3179676.
68. Warren R.M., Bashford J.A. (1981), Perception of acoustic iterance: Pitch and infrapitch, Perception and Psychophysics, 29(4): 395–402, doi: 10.3758/ BF03207350.
69. Yrttiaho S., Tiitinen H., May P.J.C., Leino S., Alku P. (2008), Cortical sensitivity to periodicity of speech sounds, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(4): 2191–2199, doi: 10.1121/1.2888489.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Carlos Jurado
1
Marcelo Larrea
1
Brian C.J. Moore
2

  1. Escuela de Ingeniería en Sonido y Acústica, Universidad de Las Américas, Avenue Granados and Colimes, EC170125, Ecuador
  2. Cambridge Hearing Group, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The present work discusses results concerning sound perception obtained in a pitch memorization experiment for blind and visually impaired subjects (children and teenagers). Listeners were divided into two age groups: 7-13 year olds and 14-18 year olds. The study tested 20 individuals (8 congenitally blind and 12 visually impaired) and 20 sighted persons comprising reference groups. The duration of the experiments was as short as possible due to the fact that our listeners were children. To date, no study has described results of such experiment for blind/visually handicapped children and teenagers. In the pitch memory experiment blind teenagers outperformed blind children and both age groups of visually impaired subjects in two out of three tested cases. These results may have implications for the development of auditory training in orientation and mobility of young visually handicapped people.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ewa Skrodzka
Edyta Bogusz
Hanna Koprowska
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This paper presents requirements for meshing of simplified type of worm gears applied in fine mechanisms. There are given some geometrical dependencies referring to meshing of the worm with the thread made of wire (having circular section). The paper describes also a computer program aiding design of such kind of worm gears.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Zdzisław Mrugalski
Wiesław Mościcki
Andrzej Odej
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The present study consisted of two experiments. The goal of the first experiment was to establish the just noticeable differences for the fundamental frequency of the vowel /u/ by using the 2AFC method. We obtained the threshold value for 27 cents. This value is larger than the motor reaction values which had been observed in previous experiments (e.g. 9 or 19 cents). The second experiment was intended to provide neurophysiological confirmation of the detection of shifts in a frequency, using event-related potentials (ERPs). We concentrated on the mismatch negativity (MMN) - the component elicited by the change in the pattern of stimuli. Its occurrence is correlated with the discrimination threshold. In our study, MMN was observed for changes greater than 27 cents - shifts of ±50 and 100 cents (effect size - Cohen’s d = 2.259). MMN did not appear for changes of ±10 and 20 cents. The results showed that the values for which motor responses can be observed are indeed lower than those for perceptual thresholds.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Małgorzata Wrzosek
Justyna Maculewicz
Honorata Hafke-Dys
Agnieszka Nowik
Anna Preis
Grzegorz Kroliczak
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The paper presents the advanced control system of the wind energy conversion with a variable speed wind turbine. The considered system consists of a wind turbine with the permanent magnet synchronous generator (PMSG), machine side converter (MSC), grid side converter (GSC) and control circuits. The mathematical models of a wind turbine system, the PMSG generator and converters have been described. The control algorithms of the converter systems based on the methods of vector control have been applied. In the advanced control system of the machine side converter the optimal MPPT control method has been used. Additionally the pitch control scheme is included in order to achieve the limitation of maximum power and to prevent mechanical damage of the wind turbine. In the control system of the grid side converter the control of active and reactive power has been applied with the application of Voltage Oriented Control (VOC). The performance of the considered wind energy system has been studied by digital simulation. The results of simulation studies confirmed the good effectiveness of the considered wind turbine system and very good performance of the proposed methods of vector control and control systems.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Piotr Gajewski
Krzysztof Pieńkowski
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

In this paper a new pitch shifter using a complex instantaneous frequency rescaler and direct digital synthesizer is presented aimed at an application in a handset calling signal composer. The pitch shifter introduced here exhibits an excellent performance as a generator of different melodies, where the sound of each note in a melody, e.g., imitating a popular hit, is derived from a short recording of a voice of a chosen creature via complex dynamic representation processing.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

E. Hermanowicz
M. Rojewski
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The aim of the study was to examine the relationship between tinnitus pitch and maximum hearing loss, frequency range of hearing loss, and the edge frequency of the audiogram, as well as, to analyze tinnitus loudness at tinnitus frequency and normal hearing frequency.

The study included 212 patients, aged between 21 to 75 years (mean age of 54.4 ± 13.5 years) with chronic subjective tinnitus and sensorineural hearing loss. For the statistical data analysis we used Chisquare test and Fisher’s exact test with level of significance p < 0:05.

Tinnitus pitch corresponding to the frequency range of hearing loss, maximum hearing loss and the edge frequency was found in 70.8%, 37.3%, and 16.5% of the patients, respectively. The majority of patients had tinnitus pitch from 3000 to 8000 Hz corresponding to the range of hearing loss (p < 0:001). The mean tinnitus pitch was 3545 Hz ± 2482. The majority (66%) of patients had tinnitus loudness 4–7 dB SL. The mean sensation level at tinnitus frequency was 4.9 dB SL ± 1.9, and 13 dB SL ± 2.9 at normal hearing frequency.

Tinnitus pitch corresponded to the frequency range of hearing loss in majority of patients. There was no relationship between tinnitus pitch and the edge frequency of the audiogram. Loudness matching outside the tinnitus frequency showed higher sensation level than loudness matching at tinnitus frequency.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Lidija Ristovska
Zora Jachova
Vase Stojcheska
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This paper is the first to optimize the friction stir welding (FSW) process considering the Clamp Pitch (mm) and Clamping Torque effect using the Various combinations of parameters were constructed using factorial design and responses, resulting in a comprehensive factorial analysis. Conspicuous changes in the tensile strength, yield strength, hardness, and power profile were observed for all amalgamations of parameters. Significant parameters of the FSW process have been considered in many optimization studies, however, the effect of the Clamp Pitch (mm) and Clamping Torque (Nm) has been never studied. Three levels of three parameters were used in the experiments: Clamp Pitch, tool rotational speed and Clamping Torque. The full factorial analysis was performed, was applied as an approach for selecting the values of the Significant factors of the parameters. For each result the three key parameters were important with p-values of less than 0.05, suggesting their significance in the phase of FSW. Mathematical models built with high R-sq. and least percentage error were adequate for the investigated responses. The findings were gained by important parameter values factors of 30 mm, 1800 rpm and 70 Nm for the take into consideration parameter range for the Clamp Pitch, rotational speed and Clamping Torque respectively.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ibrahim Sabry
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

In this study we investigate the appearance of combination tones in violins. Most authors in recent times have emphasised that combination tones occur inside the ear exclusively (intra-aural). This assumption will be subjected to scrutiny based on evidence found in an empirical study in which combination tones were measured outside the ear (extra-aural).

Measurements were performed in which a violinist played two tones of a particular musical interval simultaneously. This was recorded and subsequently analysed using a Fourier Transformation. In addition to the partial tones of the primary interval, the resulting spectrum showed frequencies corresponding to combination tones. Similar measurements on the viola and violoncello also revealed the existence of extra-aural combination tones. Such frequencies may influence the timbre of simultaneous intervals played on string instruments. In another experiment the violin was excited using an electrodynamic mini-shaker with the aim of localising the origin of extra-aural combination tones. A newly devised tone matrix was used as a theoretical approach which computes all potential combination tones that may occur between any pair of partial tones. The detailed analysis of musical intervals by both the frequency spectrum and the tone matrix shows characteristic mirror and point symmetries in the partial tone structure. The discussion focuses mainly on the audibility of extra-aural combination tones and on ‘the combination tone 1’. This research opens up new perspectives and questions relevant for interpreters, composers, violin makers and violin acousticians.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Angela Lohri
Sandra Carral
Vasileios Chatziioannou
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The paper presents a new method for building measuring instruments and systems for gyro-free determination of the parameters of moving objects. To illustrate the qualities of this method, a system for measuring the roll, pitch, heel and trim of a ship has been developed on its basis. The main concept of the method is based, on one hand, on a simplified design of the base coordinate system in the main measurement channel so as to reduce the instrumental errors, and, on the other hand, on an additional measurement channel operating in parallel with the main one and whose hardware and software platform makes possible performing algorithms intended to eliminate the dynamic error in real time. In this way, as well as by using suitable adaptive algorithms in the measurement procedures, low-cost measuring systems operating with high accuracy under conditions of inertial effects and whose parameters (intensity and frequency of the maximum in the spectrum) change within a wide range can be implemented.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Dimitar Dichev
Petr Louda
Totka Bakalova
Hristofor Koev
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Providing roughness is an effective method to heat fluids to high temperature. Present paper make use of concave dimple roughness on one and three sides of roughened ducts aimed at determining rise in heat transfer and friction of three sides over one side roughened duct. Three sides roughened duct produces high heat transfer compared to one side roughened. Results are shown as a rise in Nusselt number and friction factor of three sides over one side roughened duct. Experimental investigation was conducted under actual outdoor condition at National Institute of Technology Jamshedpur, India to test various sets of roughened collectors. Roughness parameter varied as relative roughness pitch 8–15, relative roughness height 0.018–0.045, dimple depth to diameter ratio 1–2, Reynolds number 2500–13500 at fixed aspect ratio (width/hight) 8. Highest enhancement in Nusselt number for varying relative roughness pitch, height, and diameter ratio was respectively found as 2.6 to 3.55 times, 1.91 to 3.42 times and 3.09 to 3.94 times compared to one side dimple roughened duct. Highest rise in friction for three sides over one side roughened duct for these varying parameters was respectively found as 1.62 to 2.79 times, 1.52 to 2.34 times and 2.21 to 2.56 times. To visualize the effect of roughness parameter on heat transfer and friction factor, variation in Nusselt number and friction factor for varying roughness parameters with Reynolds number is shown.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Vikash Kumar
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Flow mechanism under roughened solar air heater is quite complex. This paper is an effort towards determining the governing equations for heat transfer and friction factor for inclined spherical balls roughened ducts. With the availability of these equations, it is easier to predict the thermal and thermohydraulic performance of such roughened solar air heaters. The governing equations are derived based on the experimental data generated under actual outdoor condition at the test rig designed and fabricated at the terrace of the Mechanical Engineering Department, the National Institute of Technology Jamshedpur in India, in terms of roughness and flow parameters. Maximum augmentation in Nusselt number and friction factor for varying relative roughness pitch, relative roughness height, spherical ball height to diameter ratio, and angle of attack was respectively found to be of the order of 2.1 to 3.54 times, 1.87 to 3.21 times, 2.89 to 3.27 times and 1.74 to 3.56 times for Nusselt number and 0.84 to 1.79 times, 1.46 to 1.91 times, 1.67 to 2.34 times and 1.21 to 2.67 times for friction factor in comparison to non-roughened duct. The optimum roughness parameters under present investigation have been found.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ramesh Murmu
Parmanand Kumar
Hari N. Singh
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Artificial roughness has been found to enhance the thermal performance from the collector to air in the solar air heater duct. This paper presents the results of experimental investigation on thermal performance of three sides solar air heater roughened with combination of multiple-v and transverse wire. The range of variation of system and operating parameters is investigated within the limits of relative roughness pitch of 10−25, relative roughness height of 0.018−0.042, angle of attack of 30°−75° at varying flow Reynolds number in the of range of 3000−12000 for fixed value of relative roughness width of 6. The augmentation in fluid temperature flowing under three side’s roughened duct is found to be 36.57% more than that of one side roughened duct. The maximum thermal efficiency is obtained at relative roughness pitch of 10 and relative roughness height of 0.042, and angle of attack of 60°. The augmentation in thermal efficiency of three sides over those of one side roughened duct is found to be 46−57% for varying values of relative roughness pitch, 38−50% for varying values of relative roughness height, and 40−46% for varying values of angle of attack.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Dhananjay Kumar
Laljee Prasad
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

In the last decade, infrared imaging detectors trend has gone for smaller pixels and larger formats. Most of the time, this scaling is carried out at a given total sensitive area for a single focal plane array. As an example, QVGA 30 µm pitch and VGA 15 µm pitch exhibit exactly the same sensitive area. SXGA 10 µm pitch tends to be very similar, as well. This increase in format is beneficial to image resolution. However, this scaling to even smaller pixels raises questions because the pixel size becomes similar to the IR wavelength, but also to the typical transport dimensions in the absorbing material. Hence, maintaining resolution for such small pixel pitches requires a good control of the modulation transfer function and quantum efficiency of the array, while reducing the pixel size. This might not be obtained just by scaling the pixel dimensions. As an example, bulk planar structures suffer from excessive lateral diffusion length inducing pixel-to-pixel cross talk and thus degrading the modulation transfer function. Transport anisotropy in some type II superlattice structures might also be an issue for the diffusion modulation transfer function. On the other side, mesa structures might minimize cross talk by physically separating pixels, but also tend to degrade the quantum efficiency due to a non-negligible pixel fill factor shrinking down the pixel size. This paper discusses those issues, taking into account different material systems and structures, in the perspective of the expected future pixel pitch infrared focal plane arrays.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Olivier Gravrand 
1
Nicolas Baier 
1
Alexandre Ferron 
1
Florent Rochette  
1
Clément Lobre 
1
Jocelyn Bertoz 
2
Laurent Rubaldo
2

  1. CEA-LETI, 17 des Martyrs St., 38054 Grenoble, France
  2. Lynred, BP 21, 38113 Veurey-Voroize, France
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

In Western music culture instruments have been developed according to unique instrument acoustical features based on types of excitation, resonance, and radiation. These include the woodwind, brass, bowed and plucked string, and percussion families of instruments. On the other hand, instrument performance depends on musical training, and music listening depends on perception of instrument output. Since musical signals are easier to understand in the frequency domain than the time domain, much effort has been made to perform spectral analysis and extract salient parameters, such as spectral centroids, in order to create simplified synthesis models for musical instrument sound synthesis. Moreover, perceptual tests have been made to determine the relative importance of various parameters, such as spectral centroid variation, spectral incoherence, and spectral irregularity. It turns out that the importance of particular parameters depends on both their strengths within musical sounds as well as the robustness of their effect on perception. Methods that the author and his colleagues have used to explore timbre perception are: 1) discrimination of parameter reduction or elimination; 2) dissimilarity judgments together with multidimensional scaling; 3) informal listening to sound morphing examples. This paper discusses ramifications of this work for sound synthesis and timbre transposition.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

James Beauchamp

This page uses 'cookies'. Learn more